L’espacement extra-large des lettres améliore la lecture dans la dyslexie

1.02.2022

L’espacement extra-large des lettres améliore la lecture dans la dyslexie

Marco Zorzi , Chiara Barbiero , Andrea Facoetti , Isabella Lonciari , Marco Carrozzi , Marcella Montico , Laura Bravar , Florence George , Catherine Pech-Georgel et Johannes C. Ziegler 

Voir tous les auteurs et affiliations 

PNAS 10 juillet 2012 109 (28) 11455-11459 ; https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1205566109 

  1. Edité par Michael Posner, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, et approuvé le 23 avril 2012 (reçu pour révision le 4 avril 2012)

Résumé

Bien que les causes de la dyslexie soient encore débattues, tous les chercheurs s’accordent à dire que le principal défi est de trouver des moyens permettant à un enfant dyslexique de lire plus de mots en moins de temps, car lire plus est sans conteste l’intervention la plus efficace contre la dyslexie. Des programmes de formation sophistiqués existent, mais ils ciblent généralement les compétences constitutives de la lecture, telles que la conscience phonologique. Une fois les compétences des composants améliorées, le principal défi demeure (c’est-à-dire que les déficits en lecture doivent être traités en lisant davantage – un cercle vicieux pour un enfant dyslexique). Ici, nous montrons qu’une simple manipulation de l’espacement des lettres a considérablement amélioré les performances de lecture de texte à la volée (sans aucune formation) dans un large échantillon non sélectionné d’enfants dyslexiques italiens et français. L’espacement extra-large des lettres facilite la lecture,car les dyslexiques sont anormalement affectés par l’encombrement, un phénomène perceptif aux effets néfastes sur la reconnaissance des lettres qui est modulée par l’espacement entre les lettres. Un interlettrage extra-large peut aider à briser le cercle vicieux en rendant le matériel de lecture plus facilement accessible.

La dyslexie développementale est un trouble d’apprentissage gravement invalidant qui affecte l’acquisition de l’alphabétisation chez environ 5% de la population scolaire malgré une intelligence normale et une instruction adéquate ( 1 , 2 ). Les causes de la dyslexie font toujours l’objet de vifs débats ( 3 8 ), mais tous les chercheurs s’accordent à dire que le principal défi est la remédiation (c’est-à-dire comment amener les enfants dyslexiques à lire plus de mots en moins de temps). En effet, un enfant dyslexique lit en 1 an le même nombre de mots qu’un bon lecteur en 2 jours ( 9). L’approche la plus courante a consisté à concevoir des programmes de remédiation sophistiqués qui entraînent des sous-compétences de lecture, en particulier des compétences phonologiques et de la perception auditive. Bien que plutôt réussie, cette approche prend du temps et est difficile à mettre en œuvre dans des contextes scolaires réalistes, et les améliorations de ces sous-compétences n’améliorent pas automatiquement la lecture ( 10 , 11 ).

Une deuxième approche complémentaire consiste à se concentrer sur l’accessibilité du matériel de lecture en manipulant les propriétés physiques de l’impression (par exemple, la taille de l’impression, le type de police, etc.). * Cependant, très peu de recherches ont étudié les effets potentiels de telles manipulations ( 12 , 13 ). Nous poursuivons ici cette approche, qui est motivée par des preuves comportementales montrant que les dyslexiques sont anormalement affectés par l’encombrement ( 14 17 ), un phénomène perceptif aux effets néfastes sur la reconnaissance des lettres qui est modulé par l’espacement entre les lettres ( 18 , 19). Le surpeuplement fait référence à l’interférence des lettres d’accompagnement sur la reconnaissance des lettres cibles (revue dans les réf. 20 et 21 ). La reconnaissance est altérée lorsque les lettres sont plus proches qu’un espacement critique ( 19 ), qui est proportionnel à l’excentricité mais indépendant de la taille d’impression ( 22 ).

Le surpeuplement affecte principalement la vision périphérique chez les lecteurs adultes normaux ( 22 ), mais il affecte également la vision centrale chez les enfants d’âge scolaire ( 23 ). De plus, il existe de plus en plus de preuves que les enfants atteints de dyslexie développementale sont plus influencés par le surpeuplement que les témoins appariés selon l’âge, même dans des conditions de visualisation optimales ( 14 17 , 24 , 25 ). Il est bien connu que l’identification des lettres est une étape fondamentale de la reconnaissance visuelle des mots et de la lecture à voix haute ( 26 28 ). L’analyse d’une chaîne de lettres en ses graphèmes constitutifs est un élément clé du décodage phonologique ( 28), qui à son tour, est fondamentale pour l’acquisition de la lecture ( 29 ). Le surpeuplement peut non seulement ralentir la vitesse de lecture ( 19 , 22 ), mais également induire des erreurs de lecture, car le surpeuplement s’accompagne d’une perception confuse censée refléter la mise en commun des caractéristiques de la cible et des flankers ( 21 ). Ces résultats conduisent à la prédiction que l’espacement interlettre extra-large dans les mots devrait réduire l’encombrement et améliorer les performances de lecture chez les dyslexiques. Notez que l’espacement standard des lettres pour le texte semble être optimal chez les lecteurs adultes expérimentés. La réduction et l’augmentation de l’espacement ont toutes deux un effet néfaste sur les performances en lecture ( 18 , 19 , 22). Par exemple, la vitesse de lecture des lecteurs adultes expérimentés est ralentie lorsque l’espacement des lettres est doublé ( 19 ).

Résultats

We tested the prediction that extra-wide interletter spacing should improve reading performance in dyslexia in an unselected sample of 74 children aged between 8 and 14 y (mean age = 10.4 y, SD = 1.5), all recruited in specialized hospitals where they had been diagnosed with developmental dyslexia. The dyslexic sample included 34 Italian and 40 French children. Note that Italian has a transparent writing system, whereas French has a relatively opaque writing system, which is similar to English. The inclusion of the two languages allowed us to generalize our findings across transparent and opaque writing systems (29, 30). Children had been diagnosed as dyslexics based on standard exclusion/inclusion criteria (31). Reading performance (accuracy and/or speed) of each individual was at least 2 SDs below age-appropriate norms (Table S1).

Children had to read a text consisting of 24 short meaningful sentences that were unrelated to each other to prevent the use of contextual cues. The text was printed in black on a white A4 paper sheet using Times-Roman font and print size of 14 point (pt; 1 pt = 0.353 mm in typesetting standards). The standard interletter spacing was increased by 2.5 pt in the spaced text condition. For example, the space between i and l in the Italian word il (the) was 2.7 pt in normal text vs. 5.2 pt in spaced text. Space between words and interline spacing were also increased to maintain a proportionate appearance of the overall text (Fig. 1).

 

Fig. 1. 

Samples of the text read by the dyslexic and normally developing children matched for reading level. (A) Normal text. (B) Spaced text.

Each child was asked to read the same text in two different testing sessions, one for each condition (i.e., normal vs. spaced presentation). To minimize the effects of repetition, the sessions were separated by 2 weeks. Children in each country were randomly assigned to two groups. Group 1 was given normal text at the first time of testing (T1) and spaced text at the second time (T2). Group 2 had the opposite assignment (i.e., spaced text first and normal text second). The two groups (for both Italian and French samples) were matched for chronological age, IQ, word reading accuracy, and speed (t tests: all P values > 0.3).

Reading accuracy (number of errors) and speed (number of syllables per second) were analyzed using a mixed ANOVA with language (Italian vs. French) and group (group 1 vs. group 2) as between-subject factors and test time (T1 vs. T2) as the within-subject factor. For accuracy, the critical two-way interaction between test time and group was significant [F(1,70) = 35.16, P < 0.0001]. No other source of variance, including language, was significant (all P values > 0.18). The cross-over interaction (Fig. 2A) shows that the spaced text condition ameliorated the reading performance of dyslexic children, regardless of the order with which the spaced condition was administered. That is, regardless of whether the spaced text came first or second, dyslexics always made fewer errors on the spaced than the normal text. The manipulation of letter spacing improved accuracy in text reading by a factor of two. To fully appreciate this finding, it is important to note that the relatively low number of errors at the group level hides substantial individual differences, because some dyslexic children made virtually no errors (i.e., they were only slow), whereas others made up to 20% errors. Moreover, the sentences were short, made up of high-frequency words, and contained many monosyllabic (even one-letter) function words (determiners, conjunctions, etc.). Importantly, the benefit of letter spacing (i.e., accuracy gain) was negatively correlated with performance in a letter identification task that was administered to 20 of the French dyslexics (r = −0.60, P = 0.005). That is, the worse that children were at identifying letters, the more that they benefitted from the extra spacing.

 

Fig. 2. 

(A) Reading accuracy measured in terms of number of errors (incorrect words) as a function of group and testing time. Group 1 read normal text at the first time of testing (T1) and spaced text at the second time (T2), whereas group 2 had the opposite assignment. (B) Reading speed, in syllables per second, as a function of group and testing time. (C) Reading accuracy (number of errors) in the normal and spaced text conditions for Italian dyslexics, French dyslexics, and a younger group of Italian control children matched for reading level (RL) to the Italian dyslexic sample. (D). Reading accuracy (number of errors) for a subsample of dyslexic children that was tested a third time. Group 1 read normal text at T1, spaced text at T2, and normal text at T3, whereas group 2 had the opposite assignment. Error bars show SEM.

The analysis of reading speed showed the same two-way interaction between test time and group [F(1,70) = 27.96, P < 0.0001]. The main effect of test time (repetition) was also significant [F(1,70) = 29.37, P < 0.0001]. Children who read normal text at T1 became faster with the spaced text at T2. In contrast, children who read spaced text at T1 did not show any change in reading speed with the normal text at T2 (Fig. 2B). For the second group, the spacing benefit at T1 was as large as the repetition benefit at T2, which resulted in a flat line between T1 and T2. To get an estimate of the pure spacing benefit uncontaminated by repetition, one can compare the spacing benefit at T1 across groups. This comparison shows that spacing generated a speed improvement of about 0.3 syllables/s, which corresponds to the average improvement across 1 school year for Italian dyslexic children (32).

Separate analyses of the two dyslexic samples (Italian vs. French) confirmed the overall pattern. For accuracy, the critical two-way interaction between test time and group was the only significant effect for both samples [Italian: F(1,32) = 30.05, P < 0.0001; French: F(1,38) = 11.4, P < 0.005]. For reading speed, the data of both samples showed a significant two-way interaction [Italian: F(1,32) = 23.5, P < 0.0001; French: F(1,38) = 9.5, P < 0.005] as well as a main effect of test time [Italian: F(1,32) = 17.31, P < 0.0001; French: F(1,38) = 14.4, P < 0.005].

It is important to show that letter spacing is particularly beneficial to dyslexic children, because they are thought to suffer more from letter crowding than normally developing children. For this purpose, we compared 30 of the Italian dyslexics (mean age = 10.9 y) with a group of younger normal readers (mean age = 7.8 y) that were matched for reading level and IQ (Table S2). Note that the comparison with reading-level controls is a much more conservative and stringent test than the more typically used comparison with chronological age controls. If deficits persist with regard to reading-level controls, they are thought to reflect fundamental deficits that are not simply a consequence of the lack of reading (33). Previous studies on crowding in dyslexia have not used reading-level controls. A mixed two-way ANOVA on errors, with group as the between-subject factor and text type (normal vs. spaced) as the within-subject factor, showed a significant interaction [F(1,58) = 5.95, P = 0.018]. The effect of spacing (normal vs. spaced text) was significant for dyslexics (P < 0.001) but not for controls (P = 0.1) (Fig. 2C). The same analysis on reading speed showed a main effect of text type [F(1,58) = 10.7, P < 0.005], whereas the interaction between text type and group was only marginally significant [F(1,58) = 2.81, P = 0.09]. Nevertheless, planned contrasts showed that the effect of spacing (normal vs. spaced) was significant for dyslexics (1.75 vs. 1.94 syllables/s, P = 0.001) but not for controls (1.87 vs. 1.93 syllables/s, P > 0.3). This finding suggests that increased crowding is most likely a fundamental deficit in dyslexia that can be specifically improved by increasing interletter spacing.

To confirm the efficacy of the spacing manipulation, the sample of Italian dyslexic children was retested 2 months later (T3) using the same text condition that they were given at T1. Nine children were unavailable for the third testing session. Thus, there were 14 children in group 1 (normal–spaced–normal), and 11 children in group 2 (spaced–normal–spaced). A mixed two-way ANOVA on errors, with group as the between-subject factor and test time (T1, T2, and T3) as the within-subject factor, showed a significant interaction between group and test time [F(2,46) = 12.78, P < 0.0001; P > 0.22 for all other sources of variance]. Notably, performance of both groups at T3 was virtually identical to the respective performance at T1. Children in group 1 who showed improved performance from T1 to T2 (i.e., from normal to spaced text) returned to a higher error rate when reading normal text at T3. Conversely, group 2 decreased from T1 to T2 (i.e., from spaced to normal text) but returned to the T1 level when reading spaced text at T3 (Fig. 2D). The analysis on reading speed showed the same critical interaction between group and test time [F(2,46) = 5.4, P < 0.01] as well as the effect of repetition [i.e., main effect of test time; F(2,46) = 7.3, P < 0.005].

To test whether spacing benefits would be observed “on the fly” (i.e., within the same test session) and assess the effect on reading speed in a more stringent way (i.e., uncontaminated by repetition), we performed a second experiment on a new group of 20 Italian dyslexic children recruited using the same diagnostic criteria used in the first experiment (Table S1, experiment 2 shows sample details). We used two texts that were perfectly matched on number of words, number of syllables, word frequency, and grammatical class. This matching was achieved by replacing the content words of the text used in the previous experiment with words of the same grammatical class, word frequency, and length. Moreover, the spacing between lines in the normal text was doubled to match the line spacing of the spaced text. This spacing allowed us to assess the effect of extra-wide letter spacing disentangled from any potential contribution of the wider line spacing. Both the order of the two spacing conditions (normal vs. spaced) and the order of the two texts were fully counterbalanced across participants. Children were also tested in a second session (2 weeks apart), in which the assignment of each text to the spacing condition was reversed with respect to the first session. In the second session, the normal text had normal line spacing, which replicated the manipulation of the first experiment. Finally, to firmly exclude any potential experimenter bias, reading performance was scored offline in a double blind fashion.

The results replicated and extended our previous findings (Fig. 3). Dyslexic children made significantly fewer errors in the spaced than the normal version [6 versus 11.8; two-tailed paired t test: t(19) = 4.22, P < 0.001], and they read significantly faster in the spaced than the normal version [1.87 versus 1.64 syllables/s; t(19) = 2.16, P < 0.05]. This result shows that the spacing benefit is driven by the extra-wide letter spacing rather than the wider spacing between lines. The results were virtually identical at the second testing session, with the spaced text yielding less than one-half of the errors than the normal text [4.95 vs. 13.5; t(19) = 5.97, P < 0.001] as well as faster reading speed [2.07 vs. 1.72 syllables/s; t(19) = 3.25, P < 0.005]. Comparison of the two different texts with normal spacing across sessions showed no significant differences in both speed and accuracy (P values > 0.2), confirming that the two texts were well-matched.

 

Fig. 3. 

Experiment 2. (A) Reading accuracy measured in terms of number of errors (incorrect words) and (B) reading speed, measured in syllables per second, as a function of spacing condition and testing session. Two matched texts were administered in each testing session. The two texts in session 1 had the same (wide) line spacing, whereas in session 2, the normal text had normal line spacing (as in experiment 1). The assignment of each text to the spacing condition in session 2 was reversed with respect to session 1. Error bars show SEM.

Discussion

Overall, our results show that extra-large letter spacing provides an efficient way to improve text reading performance on the fly. We found identical effects in Italian, a shallow writing system with one-to-one correspondences between letters and sounds, and French, an opaque writing system similar to English. This finding is consistent with our hypothesis that the manipulation of spacing influences the letter identification stage, which is assumed to be identical across different alphabetic languages (28, 29). Developing a specialized neural system for visual word recognition (34) demands optimized processing to handle parallel independent identification of letters in the extreme crowding condition of printed words (35). If such optimization fails in dyslexic children, their optimal letter spacing will be increased compared with normal readers. The beneficial effect of extra-large letter spacing might also be linked to sluggish visuospatial attention in dyslexic children (7, 36). Indeed, spatial attention diminishes crowding by improving accuracy of target identification or reducing the critical spacing (21, 37). Sluggish spatial attention has recently been observed even in at-risk prereaders who later became dyslexic (38, 39).

To conclude, our findings offer a practical way to ameliorate dyslexics’ reading achievement without any training. This finding does not detract from individual remediation based on training deficient component skills. However, practitioners only know too well that getting dyslexic children to read more is a key component in achieving long-lasting improvements in reading skills. Extra-large letter spacing, which could even be optimized adaptively on an individual basis, can certainly contribute to achieving this goal.

Materials and Methods

Italian Dyslexic Children.

All Italian dyslexic children were recruited at the Institute for Maternal and Child Health “Burlo Garofolo” (Trieste, Italy). Before the study, all dyslexics received a complete medical, psychological, neuropsychological, and cognitive assessment. This assessment was done by an interdisciplinary team of psychologists, neurologists, and speech therapists. Dyslexics were included in the study if their reading performance (accuracy and/or speed) was 2 SDs below the norm on at least one of the age-standardized Italian tests included in the reading battery (40) and their global IQ was above 85 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised (WISC-R). Exclusion criteria were oral language skills in pathological range or presence of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. All children had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Accuracy and speed in word and nonword reading were measured with specific subtests of the standardized battery (40). Means and SD of all tests are presented in Table S1.

French Dyslexic Children.

Dyslexic children in France were recruited at the University Hospital La Timone Marseille, France. Before the study, all dyslexics received a complete medical, psychological, neuropsychological, and cognitive assessment by an interdisciplinary team of psychologists, neurologists, and speech therapists. Dyslexics were included if their reading performance was at least 2 SDs below the normal level on a standardized reading test (41) and their performance IQ was above 80 on the WISC—III (WISC-III). They were excluded from the study if their oral language skills were in pathological range or they were diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. All children had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Accuracy and speed in word and nonword reading were measured with a standardized computerized battery (42). Means and SD of all tests are presented in Table S1.

Italian Reading-Level Control Children.

Thirty Italian children from a public primary school in Trieste, recommended as normal readers by their teachers, were tested individually by an experimenter in a quiet classroom. Reading abilities were assessed by means of the dyslexia battery (Table S2 shows the means and SDs of all common tests across groups). The control children matched the dyslexics for reading ability, which was measured by an efficiency index calculated as the ratio between word reading speed (in seconds) and accuracy rate. The two groups were also matched for general intelligence, which was measured by WISC-R’s Similarities and Block Design subtests (for verbal and nonverbal IQ, respectively).

Materials and Procedure.

Children were asked to read aloud sentences presented as black print on an A4 paper sheet. The text had 24 short, meaningful sentences taken from the Test for Reception of Grammar (43). Sentences were unrelated to each other to prevent the use of contextual cues (e.g., “The girl had a red backpack. The bottle is bigger than the fork. The star is above the circle.”). The total number of words was 180. The text was presented on an A4 sheet that was left-justified and printed in Times-Roman font with print size of 14 pt. The standard letter spacing was increased by 2.5 pt in the spaced text condition. For example, the space between i and l in the Italian function word il was 2.7 pt in normal text vs. 5.2 pt. in spaced text. Words in the spaced text were separated by three space characters, and the interline space was doubled to maintain a proportionate appearance of the overall text. Note that Times-Roman is the most widely used font across publishers. Moreover, print size meets the recommendations (i.e., 12–14 p) of the British Dyslexia Association guidelines* on creating “dyslexia friendly” written material.

Letter Identification Task.

Twenty French dyslexic children were administered a computerized letter identification task. Children were individually tested in a quiet room and seated at a distance of 40 cm from the computer screen. At each trial, a string of five consonant letters, randomly chosen among the set (G, S, T, P, H, L) but excluding repetitions, was presented at fixation for 1,000 ms. Letters were printed in Geneva 18 bold font [height = 1.14 degrees of visual angle (deg), width = 0.86 deg], and the space between letters was 1.07 deg. Children had to identify the letter indicated by a cue—a red dot (diameter = 0.42 deg) placed below the target letter (distance = 0.54 deg) that lasted for 200 ms. Cueing positions were positions 2–4 (outer letters are easier to perceive). Children responded at the end of each trial by manually pointing to the target letter on a response screen that displayed the entire set of consonants, and the experimenter typed the response on the computer keyboard. In one-half of the trials, the cue was presented 150 ms before the onset of the letter string, and in the remaining one-half of the trials, the cue was presented at the offset of the letter string. The total number of trials was 60. For the purpose of correlating letter identification performance with spacing benefits, a letter identification score was obtained for each subject by averaging the letter identification accuracy across the six conditions (three positions by two cuing conditions) and then correlating this score with the spacing benefit, which was calculated by subtracting error rates in the normal version of the text from error rates in the spaced version of the text.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by Grant 64/05 of the Institute for Maternal and Child Health “Burlo Garofolo”, Trieste, Italy. Partial support was also provided by European Research Council Grant 210922 (to M.Z.). A.F. was supported by a grant from the University of Padova (Progetti di Ateneo 2009).

Footnotes

  • 1M.Z., C.B., and A.F. contributed equally to this work.
  • 2To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: marco.zorzi@unipd.it.
  • Author contributions: M.Z., C.B., A.F., and I.L. designed research; C.B., M.C., M.M., L.B., F.G., and C.P.-G. performed research; M.Z., C.B., A.F., and J.C.Z. analyzed data; and M.Z. and J.C.Z. wrote the paper.
  • The authors declare no conflict of interest.
  • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
  • See Commentary on page 11064
  • *The British Dyslexia Association offers specific guidelines on how to create “dyslexia friendly” written material (http://www.bdadyslexia.org.uk/about-dyslexia/further-information/dyslexia-style-guide.html). Note that letter spacing is not mentioned in the guidelines.
  • Errors were mostly lexical (i.e., misreading that produced a different word) and phonological (misreading that produced a nonword), accounting for about 95% of all errors (lexical errors were predominant—about 80% of the total). The remaining errors were mostly word omissions; line skipping and word misordering were virtually absent. The distribution of errors was similar in the normal and spaced versions of the text.
  • This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1205566109/-/DCSupplemental.

Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.

References

  1.  
    1. Snowling MJ

(2000) Dyslexia (Blackwell, Oxford). 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Shaywitz S

(2003) Overcoming Dyslexia (Vintage Books, New York). 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Gabrieli JDE

(2009) Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science 325:280–283. 

Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Ramus F

(2003) Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:212–218. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Tallal P

(2004) Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:721–728. 

PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Stein J, 
    2. Walsh V

(1997) To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci 20:147–152. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Facoetti A, 
    2. et al.

(2010) Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1011–1025. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Ziegler JC, 
    2. et al.

(2008) Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition 107:151–178. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Cunningham AE, 
    2. Stanovich KE

(1998) What reading does for the mind. Am Educ 22:8–15. 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Habib M, 
    2. et al.

(1999) Training dyslexics with acoustically modified speech: Evidence of improved phonological performance. Brain Cogn 40:143–146. 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Agnew JA, 
    2. Dorn C, 
    3. Eden GF

(2004) Effect of intensive training on auditory processing and reading skills. Brain Lang 88:21–25. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Cornelissen P, 
    2. Bradley L, 
    3. Fowler S, 
    4. Stein J

(1991) What children see affects how they read. Dev Med Child Neurol 33:755–762. 

PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. O’Brien BA, 
    2. Mansfield JS, 
    3. Legge GE

(2005) The effect of print size on reading speed in dyslexia. J Res Read 28:332–349. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Bouma H, 
    2. Legein CP

(1977) Foveal and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslexics and by average readers. Neuropsychologia 15:69–80. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Atkinson J

(1991) in Vision and Visual Dyslexia, Review of human visual development: Crowding and dyslexia, ed Stein JF (MacMillan, New York), pp 44–77. 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Spinelli- D, 
    2. De Luca M, 
    3. Judica A, 
    4. Zoccolotti P

(2002) Crowding effects on word identification in developmental dyslexia. Cortex 38:179–200. 

PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Martelli M, 
    2. Di Filippo G, 
    3. Spinelli D, 
    4. Zoccolotti P

(2009) Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. J Vis 9:14, 1–18. 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Bouma H

(1970) Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature 226:177–178. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Yu D, 
    2. Cheung SH, 
    3. Legge GE, 
    4. Chung STL

(2007) Effect of letter spacing on visual span and reading speed. J Vis 7:2.1–2.10. 

PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Pelli DG

(2008) Crowding: A cortical constraint on object recognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18:445–451. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Whitney D, 
    2. Levi DM

(2011) Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on conscious perception and object recognition. Trends Cogn Sci 15:160–168. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Pelli DG, 
    2. et al.

(2007) Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. J Vis 7:20.1–20.36. 

PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Jeon ST, 
    2. Hamid J, 
    3. Maurer D, 
    4. Lewis TL

(2010) Developmental changes during childhood in single-letter acuity and its crowding by surrounding contours. J Exp Child Psychol 107:423–437. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Geiger G, 
    2. Lettvin JY

(1987) Peripheral vision in persons with dyslexia. N Engl J Med 316:1238–1243. 

PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Lorusso ML, 
    2. et al.

(2004) Wider recognition in peripheral vision common to different subtypes of dyslexia. Vision Res 44:2413–2424. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. McClelland JL, 
    2. Rumelhart DE

(1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: 1. An account of basic findings. Psychol Rev 88:375–407. 

CrossRef Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Pelli DG, 
    2. Farell B, 
    3. Moore DC

(2003) The remarkable inefficiency of word recognition. Nature 423:752–756. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Perry C, 
    2. Ziegler JC, 
    3. Zorzi M

(2007) Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychol Rev 114:273–315. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Ziegler JC, 
    2. Goswami U

(2005) Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol Bull 131:3–29. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Ziegler JC, 
    2. et al.

(2010) Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychol Sci 21:1117–1122. 

Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. American Psychiatric Association

(1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC), 4th Ed. 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Tressoldi PE, 
    2. Stella G, 
    3. Faggella M

(2001) The development of reading speed in Italians with dyslexia: A longitudinal study. J Learn Disabil 34:414–417. 

Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Goswami U

(2003) Why theories about developmental dyslexia require developmental designs. Trends Cogn Sci 7:534–540. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. McCandliss BD, 
    2. Cohen L, 
    3. Dehaene S

(2003) The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn Sci 7:293–299. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Tydgat I, 
    2. Grainger J

(2009) Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, and symbols. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35:480–498. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Vidyasagar TR, 
    2. Pammer K

(2010) Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends Cogn Sci 14:57–63. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Yeshurun Y, 
    2. Rashal E

(2010) Precueing attention to the target location diminishes crowding and reduces the critical distance. J Vis 10:16. 

Abstract/FREE Full Text Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Facoetti A, 
    2. Corradi N, 
    3. Ruffino M, 
    4. Gori S, 
    5. Zorzi M

(2010) Visual spatial attention and speech segmentation are both impaired in preschoolers at familial risk for developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 16:226–239. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Franceschini S, 
    2. Gori S, 
    3. Ruffino M, 
    4. Pedrolli K, 
    5. Facoetti A

(2012) A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Curr Biol 22:814–819. 

CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Sartori G, 
    2. Job R, 
    3. Tressoldi PE

(1995) Battery for the Evaluation of Dyslexia and Dysorthographya (Giunti, Florence, Italy). 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Lefavrais P

(1967) Aloutette Test: Manual (Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée, Paris). 

Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Sprenger-Charolles L, 
    2. Colé P, 
    3. Béchennec D, 
    4. Kipffer-Piquard A

( 2005 ) Données normatives françaises sur la lecture et les compétences apparentées d’EVALEC, une nouvelle batterie de tests informatisés (fin Grade 1, Grade 2, Grade 3 et Grade 4) Rev Eur Psychologie Appliquée 55 : 157 – 186 . 

RéfCroisée Google Scholar

  1.  
    1. Évêque DVM

( 1982 ) Test de réception de grammaire ( Conseil de recherches médicales , Oxford ).